All insects are equal, but some insects are more equal than others

Arnout R.H. Fischer (Wageningen University, Wageningen, The Netherlands)
L.P.A. (Bea) Steenbekkers (Wageningen University, Wageningen, The Netherlands)

British Food Journal

ISSN: 0007-070X

Article publication date: 3 April 2018

3230

Abstract

Purpose

Lack of acceptance of insects as food is considered a barrier against societal adoption of the potentially valuable contribution of insects to human foods. An underlying barrier may be that insects are lumped together as one group, while consumers typically try specific insects. The purpose of this paper is to investigate the ways in which Dutch consumers, with and without insect tasting experience, are more or less willing to eat different insects.

Design/methodology/approach

In a quasi-experimental study (n=140), the participants with and without prior experience in eating insects were asked to give their willingness to eat a range of insects, and their attitudes and disgust towards eating insects.

Findings

Insects promoted in the market were more preferred than the less marketed insects, and a subgroup of preferred insects for participants with experience in eating insects was formed.

Research limitations/implications

Although well-known insects were more preferred, general willingness to eat remained low for all participants. The results indicate that in future research on insects as food the specific insects used should be taken into account.

Practical implications

Continued promotion of specific, carefully targeted, insects may not lead to short-term uptake of insects as food, but may contribute to willingness to eat insects as human food in the long term.

Originality/value

The paper shows substantial differences between consumers who have and who have not previously tasted insects, with higher acceptance of people with experience in tasting insects for the specific insects that are frequently promoted beyond their generally more positive attitude towards eating insects.

Keywords

Citation

Fischer, A.R.H. and Steenbekkers, L.P.A.(B). (2018), "All insects are equal, but some insects are more equal than others", British Food Journal, Vol. 120 No. 4, pp. 852-863. https://doi.org/10.1108/BFJ-05-2017-0267

Publisher

:

Emerald Publishing Limited

Copyright © 2018, Arnout R.H. Fischer and L.P.A. (Bea) Steenbekkers

License

Published by Emerald Publishing Limited. This article is published under the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY 4.0) licence. Anyone may reproduce, distribute, translate and create derivative works of this article (for both commercial and non-commercial purposes), subject to full attribution to the original publication and authors. The full terms of this licence may be seen at http://creativecommons.org/licences/by/4.0/legalcode


Introduction

In the next few decades, the growing world population and increasing societal welfare will double the demand for high-quality proteins for human consumption; while at the same time, the ecological footprint of the current food production needs to be halved to ensure long-term sustainability. As a consequence, traditional livestock production needs to be supplemented as a main source of protein for human consumption by other sources of protein (Boland et al., 2013). Insects typically contain from 30 to 81 per cent protein per 100 grams dry-weight; which is comparable to protein contents of meats such as beef or pork. Insects are also rich in essential amino acids and unsaturated fatty acids. Some insects contain important (trace) minerals and vitamins in quantities higher than in most animal or plant food products (Ramos-Elorduy, 2009). For example, termites are rich in calcium and sulphur whereas grasshoppers are rich in iron and zinc (Ramos-Elorduy, 2009). Insects are also generally rich in niacin, riboflavin and thiamine (Rumpold and Schlüter, 2013). Insects require less energy and space to grow than meat-producing animals (Van Huis et al., 2013). Food conversion rates of insects are better than even efficient meat sources like poultry (Rumpold and Schlüter, 2013). Food waste can, in some situations, be used to breed insects, although this is not yet allowed in the EU (Finke et al., 2015). There are close to 2,000 edible insects which include beetles, butterflies, moths, wasps, bees and ants (Ramos-Elorduy, 2009). In addition, insects are eaten in various stages of maturity; for example mealworms, the larvae stage of the mealworm beetle, are consumed, while mature crickets and locusts are eaten (Zielińska et al., 2015). Together these properties make insects a potential sustainable supplement to the existing protein sources for human nutrition.

However, for this potential to be reached the insects and/or insect products need to be consumed. Entomophagy or purposely eating insects is found in about 130 countries, but is not common in European and American industrialised cultures (Van Huis et al., 2013). In those cultures, insects as a food source have long been limited to the menu of certain niche restaurants and to small-scale companies and retailers selling packaged, often freeze dried, insects for consumption. More recently, insect tastings (Shelomi, 2015) and some limited sales in supermarkets have aimed at larger scale introductions of insects as food in European countries such as the Netherlands (House, 2016). In particular mealworms, crickets and grasshoppers were introduced to the Dutch consumers through tastings and web-shops[1] and via insect cookbooks (e.g. Van Huis et al., 2014).

Nevertheless, published studies in Europe show low acceptance across the continent. Members of the Dutch public have a low expected liking of insect foods (Tan, Fischer, Van Trijp and Stieger, 2016), an aversion to eating insects and a low acceptance of insects as food source (Schösler et al., 2012, Looy et al., 2014). The aversion to eating insects is differentiated across the population, with some segments of the Belgian population in particular those showing less neophobia being more open to the consumption of insects than others (Verbeke, 2015), and German consumers with a higher level of neophobia being less willing to eat insects (Hartmann et al., 2015), suggesting neophobia as a relevant predictor to acceptance of insect eating.

Much of the research has been done on the appropriateness of the product in which insects are invisibly used both in European (Tan et al., 2017; Gmuer et al., 2016) but also in African countries (Alemu et al., 2017). Retail introductions of insect burgers have followed such a product-centred approach by introducing insect burgers containing invisible insects (House, 2016). This is based on the finding that disgust is lower when insect protein is applied as invisible, ground ingredient (Caparros Megido et al., 2016). A study using crickets among Swiss participants showed that, at least in that case, it was the inappropriate combination of visible crickets with known food products that resulted in very negative responses, even more strongly so than the crickets presented as standalone food (Gmuer et al., 2016). While the approach of investigating insect acceptance as food in general, or focussing on specific insects certainly has value, it may lead to overgeneralisation of conclusions if consumers have systematically different evaluations of different insects. The current paper aims to provide insight into how different insects evoke different levels of willingness to eat these insects by consumers.

Disgust (Looy et al., 2014), considerations of insects as being inappropriate for human consumption (Tan, Fischer, Van Trijp and Stieger, 2016) or inappropriately being added to the existing foods (Gmuer et al., 2016; de-Magistris et al., 2015) are raised as barriers to the willingness to eat insects. Some studies suggest that these barriers depend on inborn disgust and that this creates a lasting negative affective attitude towards eating insects in general and that only cultural reinforcement can make eating insects accepted (Ruby et al., 2015). Other research does argue that the disgust evoked towards insect eating may be the learned cultural response (Deroy et al., 2015). Regardless of whether disgust is inborn or learnt, a distinction seems to be made between more and less accepted insects, which can be found among populations in Africa that consume specific insects, while neighbouring populations (with access to the same wildlife) do not consume those same insects (Van Huis, 2003). People from a Thai region in which entomophagy is common, most appreciated insects that were food ingredients in their cuisine and occurred locally, but were disgusted by the mealworms which they associated with spoilage (Tan et al., 2015). While these studies focus on the cultures where insect eating is fairly common, a study in a related field shows that the Dutch people do take due account of the species of animals when deciding whether a product is acceptable to eat (Bekker, Tobi and Fischer, 2017), and results from Tan et al. (2015) show that the preference order of insects among the Thai people differed from that of the Dutch with prior experience in insect tasting. Thai people were in fact more disgusted by mealworms than the Dutch people with prior experience. In a comparative study between German and Chinese consumers, it was found that Chinese consumers do distinguish between crickets and culturally known silkworms, while Germans do not (Hartmann et al., 2015). Other research showed that among Czech consumers, some insects (cockroaches) evoke more disgust than others (crickets) (Bednárová et al., 2013). This suggests that even in countries were insect eating is not common, consumers distinguish between different insects, but that experience with specific insects matter in the extent consumers are willing to accept these specific insects as food.

In spite of these different practices, much of the current debate about entomophagy is about insect eating in general (Evans et al., 2015), i.e. focussing on a single overarching category of insects. From a consumer point of view, categorisation is a way to make sense of a product in the given context by placing it in a category that provides maximal fit with other category members (Mervis and Rosch, 1981). While it makes sense to place insects in a global category if we merely see them as animals, if consumers consider insects in the perspective of food, a global taxonomic insect category may not suffice, and a different, goal based category is more relevant (cf. Huffman and Houston, 1993). In such cases where global categories do not fit a use context consumers tend to create subcategories to interpret specific products from the global category in the goal context (cf. Sujan and Dekleva, 1987; Ratneshwar and Shocker, 1991). This leads to different inferences of product properties (Redden, 2008) which in the case of insects can lead to a distinctly different appreciation of the subcategory of food insects. The creation of subcategories requires some experience in order to reconsider specific products as member of a specific subcategory (Johnson and Mervis, 1997; Tanaka and Taylor, 1991). In the case of insects, we would therefore expect that a particular group of insects that is more consistently shown as food in specific recipes (e.g. as in Van Huis et al., 2014), and that is presented as food to and tasted by the public would be subcategorised as food insects, and in particular by those consumers that have at least some experience in eating them.

Therefore, we hypothesise that for people with no prior experience in eating insects, preferences towards eating specific insects will follow more or less a continuum from very unfavourable to unfavourable, while for those who do have experience in eating insects, the insects most frequently available as food in the region inhabited by the consumer will stand out as a more distinct and more preferred subcategory.

Method

The study included a two level (no prior experience-prior experience with eating insects) quasi-experimental factor. In addition, 17 different insects were rated on willingness to eat by each participant. To test the hypothesis that insects known as food would be subcategorised as food insects, the three insects most generally presented to the public in tastings, insects festivals and retail in the Netherlands (mealworm, cricket, grasshopper)[2] were included. To test a broad view across edible insects, these were presented as part of a set of 17 insects consisting of more broadly liked insects (grasshopper, butterfly, dragonfly, caterpillar) more neutral or ambivalently regarded insects (cricket, beetle, moth, bee) infrequently mentioned insects (termite, worm, water bug) and generally disliked insects (cockroach, ant, wasp) (Shipley and Bixler, 2017). In addition, we included insect eggs, and included a subfamily of grasshopper: slantface (Acridinae) to include a largely unknown insect name as control.

Participants were invited through leaflets and internet forums or social media to participate in a study on insect eating. The participants were students at the Wageningen University. Wageningen has hosted several insect tasting trials, and entomophagy is taught and studied in several departments at the Wageningen University, which makes it more likely to identify the participants who had experience in eating insects than when using the general population. Sampling included an active search for the participants with experience in eating insects. Data were collected in 2014. The final data set consisted of 140 surveys[3]. Of the participants, 54 per cent (76) were female, 36 per cent male (50) and 10 per cent (14) did not report their gender. Average age was 24.9 years (SD=7.2). Slightly more than half (n=77) had not eaten insects and 45 per cent (n=63) had (deliberately) eaten insects.

Measures

Willingness to eat an individual insect was measured on a single five-point item “To what extent would you be willing to eat (named insect)” ranging from “1: very unlikely” to “5: very likely”. In addition, general attitude towards eating insects was measured with a reliable 19-item, seven-point semantic differential attitude scale (adapted from Crites et al., 1994) containing a general attitude towards eating insects subscale (four items, Cronbach’s α=0.93) an affective attitude subscale (seven items, Cronbach’s α=0.94) and a cognitive attitude subscale (eight items, Cronbach’s α=0.88) (previously used in a similar context by Bekker, Fischer, Tobi and Van Trijp, 2017). In addition, the general acceptance of insects as food was measured with a nine-item seven-point Likert scale (Cronbach’s α=0.90) (adapted from Schutz, 1965). General disgust towards eating insects was measured with a six-item seven-point Likert scale (Cronbach’s α=0.91) (adapted from Rozin and Fallon, 1980). Food neophobia was also measured using an eight-item seven-point Likert scale (Cronbach’s α=0.81) (adapted from Pliner and Hobden, 1992). An overview of all items is provided in the Appendix. As all Cronbach αs were above the recommended limit of 0.70 (Nunnally, 1978), the scales were deemed reliable and average item scores were calculated to represent the measured constructs. After a pre-test with ten participants to determine whether questions were understandable, no adjustments were needed.

Procedure

The participants were mailed a link and could complete the questionnaire from any internet connection at their own timing. When following the link they were first introduced to the research. They then received the information that “Insects are eaten in 130 countries in the world, there are over 2,000 species of insects that can be eaten”.

The participants then answered whether they had ever (deliberately) eaten insects themselves, followed by rating their attitude towards eating insects in general. They then filled out general acceptance of insects as foods, disgust and neophobia items, following by their willingness to eat the individual insects. The study ended with questions about age, gender and study programmes, after which the participants were thanked for their involvement in the study.

Results

Of those who had tried insects before (n=63), 68 per cent (43) reported they would be willing to eat them again, 5 per cent (three) would not be willing to eat them again and 27 per cent (17) were not yet sure. Almost all participants with experience in eating insects (98 per cent) reported their insect consumption was rare and was at most a few times per year.

Disgust, general, affective and cognitive attitude towards and acceptance of insects as food and neophobia were all significantly correlated in the expected direction (Table I). A logistic regression showed that the likelihood of having experience with eating insects depends on neophobia (χ2(1)=5.02; p=0.03; Nagelkerke R2=0.05, odds ratio: 1.51), to the extent that people with higher neophobia less frequently had experience with eating insects.

A multivariate analysis of variance showed that the participants with experience in eating insects were significantly more positive in overall attitude (F(1,130)=31.55, p<0.001, η2=0.20), cognitive attitude (F(1,133)=28.90, p<0.001, η2=0.18), affective attitude (F(1,138)=48.14, p<0.001, η2=0.26) and acceptance of insects as foods (F(1,127)=34.71, p<0.001, partial η2=0.21). Insect-eating people scored lower on disgust (F(1,127)=28.31, p<0.001, partial η2=0.18) (Figure 1).

A repeated measures ANOVA was conducted with the willingness to eat each of the 17 insects as the repeated factor and the quasi-experimental factor whether a participant had experience with eating insects. The analysis confirmed that the participants with experience with eating insects scored on average more positive towards the insects (F(1,121)=27.04, p<0.001, partial η2=0.18). More importantly the repeated measures analysis revealed that not all insects were liked the same (F(1,121)=36.19, p<0.001, partial η2=0.23 (lower bound)); and, in line with our hypothesis the interaction between the repeated factor (insect) and whether participants had experience with eating was significant (F(1,121)=5.25, p=0.02, partial η2=0.04). Simple effects analysis showed that the differences between insects were significant for both those with experience (F(16,106)=10.04, p<0.01, partial η2=0.60,range of 2.01 scale point) and for those without experience with eating insects (F(16,106)=2.90, p<0.01, partial η2=0.31, range of 1.09 scale point) (Figure 2).

After establishing that the participants with experience in eating insects more clearly distinguished between insects in their willingness to eat insects than those who have no experience, we focused on the individual insects (Table II). Pairwise comparisons between specific insects were made (Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons). The participants with experience in eating insects showed a distinct subcategory of most favoured insects, being those insects most frequently used in tastings (mealworms, crickets and grasshoppers). In addition, a substantial number of insects not readily available for consumption (dragonfly, eggs, water bugs, worms, termites, ants, caterpillars and beetles) were both significantly and relevantly favoured (as evidenced by partial η2 above 0.09) by the participants with experience with eating insects over participants without experience. For the more negatively rated butterfly, moth, bee, wasp and cockroach participants with experience with eating insects were significantly more willing to eat those compared to participants without experience, but these differences were small (as evidenced by partial η2 between 0.01 and 0.09); indicating that the difference between eaters and non-eaters was less pronounced for these insects.

Using a general linear model, with the 17 insects as repeated measure and overall attitude, affective attitude, cognitive attitude, general acceptance of insects as food and neophobia as predictor, showed similar regression coefficients for each predictor regardless of the specific insect (all Fs<1.6, all ps⩾0.10). A subsequent multiple regression analysis on the aggregated willingness to eat across all 17 insects only showed effects of disgust and affective attitude on willingness to eat insects (Table III). Adding the interaction effects of those who had previously consumed insects or not with the predictors was not statistically significant (Fchange(6,112)=0.60, p=0.73). The noticeable lack of effect of neophobia suggests that the effect of neophobia is mediated through attitude components rather than being a proximal effect on willingness to eat insects.

Some caution is warranted in interpreting the analyses reported in Table III, as collinearity statistics for the different attitude components, including overall acceptance of insects as food, are only just acceptable. What can be concluded is that emotional responses (affective attitude component and disgust) are the strongest predictors of willingness to eat insects, and that in this case where attitudes and disgust are included neophobia has limited to no additional explanatory value for willingness to eat insects.

Conclusions and discussion

This study confirms and combines findings from the previous studies on European samples by showing that those with experience in eating insects before show a higher willingness to eat insects again (as shown by Tan, Van Den Berg and Stieger, 2016), and that there are significant differences in willingness to eat different insects (as shown by Zielińska et al., 2015). This suggests that disgust and attitude towards the idea of eating insects in general is not the only factor predicting the willingness to eat specific insects. The four insects the participants were least willing to eat; wasp, cockroach, bee and moth were listed elsewhere as disliked (cockroach, wasp), or ambivalent (bee, moth) (Shipley and Bixler, 2017). The insects (grasshoppers, crickets and mealworms), most often marketed as food in the culture of the participants, were those they are most willing to eat. These results align with the results by an earlier study in a Thai insect-eating culture (Tan et al., 2015) where the participants were not willing to eat insects not readily available for consumption within that culture, even when these insects are consumed in other cultures. Our findings thus suggest that trying specific insects influences the willingness to eat those specific insects more than the willingness to eat an insect in general. A further indication for this subcategorization can be found in the observation that the insects most frequently marketed as food (cricket, grasshopper, mealworm) formed a significantly distinct group of insects with the highest willingness to eat, that stood apart from the other insects for participants with experience in eating insects, but not for those without such experience. Similar sub-categorisation may also influence the introduction of other new foods such as algae or meat replacers.

General disgust and the general affective attitude component most frequently had a (negative) effect on willingness to eat a specific insect. The finding that disgust is an important predictor of (non)willingness to eat insects replicates the existing research (e.g. Looy et al., 2014). Where Gmuer et al. (2016) similarly show that more emotions than only disgust matter in willingness to eat insects, the current research adds to that, that affective attitude contributes to willingness to eat beyond more cognition-based evaluations. Thus, our results further emphasise that willingness to eat insects is driven to a large extent by emotions.

The finding in the current study that neophobia did not significantly contribute to willingness to eat insects seems at odds with other research in willingness to eat insects that found significant and substantial contributions of neophobia to willingness to eat insects (Hartmann et al., 2015; Verbeke, 2015). In other studies, much smaller effects of neophobia where found however (Tan, Fischer, Van Trijp and Stieger, 2016; Tan, Van Den Berg and Stieger, 2016). If we compare these studies, it is noticeable that the studies of Hartmann and colleagues and Verbeke do not include any evaluation of eating insects as predictor of willingness to eat insects, while the other studies included evaluations of insect products besides neophobia, i.e. of perceived appropriateness (Tan, Fischer, Van Trijp and Stieger, 2016) and anticipated taste (Tan, Van Den Berg and Stieger, 2016). The current study includes a measure of evaluation (i.e. attitude as defined in e.g. Eagly and Chaiken, 1993) of insects as food. Therefore, we argue that neophobia is not a proximal determinant of willingness to eat insects but rather a further removed determinant that exerts its influence through perceptions and evaluations. This makes sense if we consider that neophobia is a personality trait (Pliner and Hobden, 1992) and therefore likely to be an antecedent for all perceptions and evaluations in the mind. Future research should focus on how to model neophobia in more complex causal models both within the context of eating insects, but also for other new foods.

A practical implication and limitation is that although there were clear differences between those with and without experience with eating insects, willingness to eat insects was fairly limited even among those who had previously eaten insects. In addition, we should take into account that reported willingness to eat tends to be higher than the acceptance of insects as part of the regular diet (Tan, Fischer, Van Trijp and Stieger, 2016). Therefore, the willingness to include insects in the regular diet is probably even lower than the estimates in this study. The sample consisted of volunteers who were invited to a study on insect eating. In addition, the sample mainly consisted of the students of Wageningen University, which has been one of the centres of entomophagy in Europe which may have favoured their opinion about eating insects. This may have somewhat biased the sample towards the participants more than averagely positive about the idea of insects eating, and this would likely increase differences between those who had experience with eating insects (and thus shown a somewhat positive attitude towards insects eating) and those who had not. Future research should investigate to what extent the current results can be generalised. In combination with other, practical barriers identified in other research, such as the lack of understanding how to prepare and what to expect of the taste of whole insects (Tan et al., 2015), and limited availability of insect products (House, 2016; Shelomi, 2015), the shown low willingness to eat insects even for those who had eaten insects before shows that acceptance of insects as food remains problematic.

The current results do suggest that future effort to promote insects as food should include a systematic approach to identifying which insects are suitable for continued introduction in Europe. The current study shows that the introduction of specific insects as food may have resulted in the reduction of negative sentiment to eating at least some insects, as particularly people who already reported experience with eating insects showed distinctly higher willingness to eat these insects. This success remains limited at best, however, as willingness to eat even those insects was fairly low. Nevertheless, continued investment in promoting specific insects besides overcoming other practical barriers identified in other research may contribute to higher willingness to eat insects in the future.

For now, we would suggest that at least in the Netherlands, and at this moment in time promotion efforts for grasshoppers, mealworms and crickets – the insects highest scoring on willingness to eat – may be the best way forward in the short term.

Figures

Means and 95 per cent confidence interval (measured on seven-point scales) for the constructs related to response towards eating insects in general for those with and without prior experience in eating insects

Figure 1

Means and 95 per cent confidence interval (measured on seven-point scales) for the constructs related to response towards eating insects in general for those with and without prior experience in eating insects

Mean (95 per cent confidence interval) of willingness to eat (1=very unlikely; 5=very likely) specific insects for participants who had, and had not prior experience with eating insects. Insects marked (+) were available for human consumption in the Netherlands at the time of data collection

Figure 2

Mean (95 per cent confidence interval) of willingness to eat (1=very unlikely; 5=very likely) specific insects for participants who had, and had not prior experience with eating insects. Insects marked (+) were available for human consumption in the Netherlands at the time of data collection

Pearson correlations between attitude, acceptance, disgust and neophobia

Cognitive attitude Affective attitude Acceptance Disgust Neophobia
Overall attitude 0.87** 0.83** 0.77** −0.73** −0.41**
Cognitive attitude 0.78** 0.67** −0.64** −0.40**
Affective attitude 0.76** −0.70** −0.41**
Acceptance −0.79** −0.43**
Disgust 0.42**

Notes: *p<0.05; **p<0.01

Comparison between willingness to eat (on a scale from 1 – very unlikely, to 5 – very likely) specific insects between participants with and without experience in eating insects

Experience (n=56) No Experience (n=67) Comparison between experienced and non-experienced eaters
Mean (95% CI) Mean (95% CI) Mean difference Significance Partial η2
Wasp 1.95 (1.68, 2.21)a 1.49 (1.25, 1.74)a 0.45 0.014 0.05
Cockroach 2.04 (1.74, 2.33)ab 1.52 (1.25, 1.79)ab 0.51 0.013 0.05
Bee 2.13 (1.82, 2.43)abc 1.61 (1.33, 1.89)abc 0.51 0.015 0.05
Moth 2.32 (2.00, 2.64)abcd 1.75 (1.45, 2.04)abc 0.58 0.010 0.05
Butterfly 2.57 (2.24, 2.90)bcde 1.78 (1.47, 2.08)abc 0.80 0.001 0.09
Dragonfly 2.66 (2.33, 2.99)cde 1.87 (1.56, 2.17)abc 0.80 <0.001 0.20
Slantface 2.82 (2.53, 3.12)e 1.72 (1.45, 1.99)abc 1.11 0.001 0.09
Eggs 2.75 (2.41, 3.09)de 1.82 (1.51, 2.13)abc 0.93 <0.001 0.12
Water bugs 2.79 (2.48, 3.09)de 1.82 (1.54, 2.10)abc 0.96 <0.001 0.15
Worms 2.80 (2.47, 3.14)de 1.85 (1.55, 2.15)abc 0.95 <0.001 0.15
Termites 2.86 (2.52, 3.19)e 1.81 (1.50, 2.11)abc 1.05 <0.001 0.13
Ants 3.05 (2.72, 3.39)e 1.84 (1.53, 2.15)abc 1.22 <0.001 0.19
Caterpillars 2.93 (2.57, 3.28)e 2.10 (1.78, 2.43)cd 0.82 <0.001 0.09
Beetles 3.09 (2.73, 3.44)e 2.00 (1.67, 2.33)bcd 1.09 <0.001 0.14
Mealworms (+) 3.84 (3.48, 4.19)f 2.19 (1.87, 2.52)cde 1.65 <0.001 0.28
Crickets (+) 3.66 (3.27, 4.05)f 2.42 (2.06, 2.77)de 1.24 <0.001 0.15
Grasshoppers (+) 3.96 (3.58, 4.35)f 2.58 (2.23, 2.94)e 1.38 <0.001 0.18
Average across all insects 2.84 (2.57, 3.10) 1.89 (1.65, 2.13) 0.95 <0.001 0.18

Notes: Cells within the same column sharing a superscript character are not significantly different at p=0.05 (Bonferroni correction). Insects names marked (+) were available for human consumption in the Netherlands at the time of data collection

Regression weights for multiple regressions of determinants of the average willingness to eat specific insects (on a scale from 1 – very unlikely, to 5 – very likely)

95% CI
Regression coefficient Lower bound Upper bound Standardised regression coefficient Tolerance (collinearity statistic)a t (118) p
Constant 1.49 0.42 2.57
Overall attitude 0.13 −0.07 0.32 0.19 0.10 1.24 0.22
Cognitive attitude −0.05 −0.21 0.11 −0.06 0.20 −0.58 0.56
Affective attitude 0.26 0.04 0.48 0.24 0.22 2.35 0.02
Acceptance of insects as food in general 0.12 −0.06 0.31 0.19 0.15 1.31 0.19
Disgust with eating insects −0.25 −0.36 −0.15 −0.39 0.35 −4.93 <0.001
Food neophobia −0.019 −0.17 0.13 −0.01 0.74 −0.25 0.81
Experience with eating insects (−0.5: no, +0.5: yes) 0.04 −0.21 0.29 0.02 0.72 0.32 0.75

Notes: aTolerance does not surpass the critical limit of 0.10 and overall attitude is therefore maintained; Model 2 – not reported shows no significant contribution of two-way interaction terms between previous insects’ consumers and the other predictors. Overall model: F(7,118)=47.08, p<0.01; R2=0.74. Model 2: F(13,112)=25.12, p<0.01; R2=0.74; F-test for significant increase in R2 (H0: no change): Fchange (6,112)=0.60, pchange=0.73

Notes

1.
3.

In total, 17 incomplete surveys were encountered which were used for analyses if applicable resulting in some variations in n and dfs in the results section.

Appendix: Scale items

All items measured on seven-point scales. (R) indicates recoded items. English version of the used items. The Dutch version used in the research is available from the first author on request.

Adapted from Crites et al. (1994), “When I think I have to eat insects I feel:” Affect: hateful-love; sad-delighted; annoyed-happy; tense-calm; bored-excited; angry-relaxed; disgusted-acceptance; sorrow-joy. Cognition: useless-useful; foolish-wise; unsafe-safe; harmful-beneficial; worthless-valuable; imperfect-perfect; unhealthy-healthy. General: negative-positive; dislike-like; bad-good; undesirable-desirable.

Adapted from Schutz (1965), general acceptance towards eating insects (disagree completely-agree completely): “I would eat insects every opportunity I have”; “I would eat insects if available but would not go out of my way”; “I would eat insects only if there were no other food choices”(R); “I would eat insects in the future”; “I like insects and would eat them now and then”; “ I don’t like the taste, smell or texture of insects” (R); “I do not like the appearance of insects” (R); “I will never eat insects” (R).

Adapted from Rozin and Fallon (1980), the disgust towards eating insects (disagree completely-agree completely): “The thought of eating insects makes me nauseous”; ”I dislike the idea of having insects in my stomach”; “I would dislike it if insect food got on my hands”; “I would dislike any dish that contained even the tiniest amount of insects”; “I dislike insect food because of the idea what it is or where it comes from”; “I feel that insect food might contain something that might physically endanger my body”.

Adapted from Pliner and Hobden (1992), food neophobia (disagree completely-agree completely): “I am constantly sampling new and different foods” (R); “I don’t trust new foods”; “I like foods from different countries” (R); “ethnic food looks too weird to eat”; “at dinner parties, I will try a new food” (R); “I am very particular about the foods I will eat”; “I will eat almost anything” (R); “ I like to try new ethnic restaurants” (R).

References

Alemu, M.H., Olsen, S.B., Vedel, S.E., Pambo, K.O. and Owino, V.O. (2017), “Combining product attributes with recommendation and shopping location attributes to assess consumer preferences for insect-based food products”, Food Quality and Preference, Vol. 55, January, pp. 45-57.

Bednárová, M., Borkovcová, M., Mlcek, J., Rop, O. and Zeman, L. (2013), “Edible insects – species suitable for entomophagy under condition of Czech Republic”, Acta Universitatis Agriculturae et Silviculturae Mendelianae Brunensis, Vol. 61 No. 3, pp. 587-593.

Bekker, G.A., Tobi, H. and Fischer, A.R.H. (2017), “Meet meat: an explorative study on meat and cultured meat as seen by Chinese, Ethiopians and Dutch”, Appetite, Vol. 114, July, pp. 82-92.

Bekker, G.A., Fischer, A.R.H., Tobi, H. and Van Trijp, H.C.M. (2017), “Explicit and implicit attitude toward an emerging food technology: the case of cultured meat”, Appetite, Vol. 108, January, pp. 245-254.

Boland, M.J., Rae, A.N., Vereijken, J.M., Meuwissen, M.P.M., Fischer, A.R.H., Van Boekel, M.A.J.S., Rutherfurd, S.M., Gruppen, H., Moughan, P.J. and Hendriks, W.H. (2013), “The future supply of animal-derived protein for human consumption”, Trends in Food Science and Technology, Vol. 29 No. 1, pp. 62-73.

Caparros Megido, R., Gierts, C., Blecker, C., Brostaux, Y., Haubruge, É., Alabi, T. and Francis, F. (2016), “Consumer acceptance of insect-based alternative meat products in Western countries”, Food Quality and Preference, Vol. 52, May, pp. 237-243.

Crites, S.L., Fabrigar, L.R. and Petty, R.E. (1994), “Measuring the affective and cognitive properties of attitudes: conceptual and methodological issues”, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Vol. 20 No. 6, pp. 619-634.

De-Magistris, T., Pascucci, S. and Mitsopoulos, D. (2015), “Paying to see a bug on my food: how regulations and information can hamper radical innovations in the European Union”, British Food Journal, Vol. 117 No. 6, pp. 1777-1792.

Deroy, O., Reade, B. and Spence, C. (2015), “The insectivore’s dilemma, and how to take the West out of it”, Food Quality and Preference, Vol. 44, September, pp. 44-55.

Eagly, A.H. and Chaiken, S. (1993), The Psychology of Attitudes, Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Fort Worth, TX.

Evans, J., Alemu, M., Flore, R., Frøst, M., Halloran, A., Jensen, A., Maciel-Vergara, G., Meyer-Rochow, V., Münke-Svendsen, C. and Olsen, S. (2015), “‘Entomophagy’: an evolving terminology in need of review”, Journal of Insects as Food and Feed, Vol. 1 No. 4, pp. 293-305.

Finke, M.D., Rojo, S., Roos, N., Van Huis, A. and Yen, A.L. (2015), “The European Food Safety Authority scientific opinion on a risk profile related to production and consumption of insects as food and feed”, Journal of Insects as Food and Feed, Vol. 1 No. 4, pp. 245-247.

Gmuer, A., Nuessli Guth, J., Hartmann, C. and Siegrist, M. (2016), “Effects of the degree of processing of insect ingredients in snacks on expected emotional experiences and willingness to eat”, Food Quality and Preference, Vol. 54, December, pp. 117-127.

Hartmann, C., Shi, J., Giusto, A. and Siegrist, M. (2015), “The psychology of eating insects: a cross-cultural comparison between Germany and China”, Food Quality and Preference, Vol. 44, September, pp. 148-156.

House, J. (2016), “Consumer acceptance of insect-based foods in the Netherlands: academic and commercial implications”, Appetite, Vol. 107, December, pp. 47-58.

Huffman, C. and Houston, M.J. (1993), “Goal-oriented experiences and the development of knowledge”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 20 No. 2, pp. 190-207.

Johnson, K.E. and Mervis, C.B. (1997), “Effects of varying levels of expertise on the basic level of categorization”, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, Vol. 126 No. 3, pp. 248-277.

Looy, H., Dunkel, F.V. and Wood, J.R. (2014), “How then shall we eat? Insect-eating attitudes and sustainable foodways”, Agriculture and Human Values, Vol. 31 No. 1, pp. 131-141.

Mervis, C.B. and Rosch, E. (1981), “Categorization of natural objects”, Annual Review of Psychology, Vol. 32, pp. 89-115.

Nunnally, J.C. (1978), Psychometric Theory, McGraw-Hill, New York, NY.

Pliner, P. and Hobden, K. (1992), “Development of a scale to measure the trait of food neophobia in humans”, Appetite, Vol. 19 No. 2, pp. 105-120.

Ramos-Elorduy, J. (2009), “Anthropo-entomophagy: cultures, evolution and sustainability”, Entomological Research, Vol. 39 No. 5, pp. 271-288.

Ratneshwar, S. and Shocker, A.D. (1991), “Substitution in use and the role of usage context in product category structures”, Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 28 No. 3, pp. 281-295.

Redden, J.P. (2008), “Reducing satiation: the role of categorization level”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 34 No. 5, pp. 624-634.

Rozin, P. and Fallon, A. (1980), “The psychological categorization of foods and non-foods: a preliminary taxonomy of food rejections”, Appetite, Vol. 1 No. 3, pp. 193-201.

Ruby, M.B., Rozin, P. and Chan, C. (2015), “Determinants of willingness to eat insects in the USA and India”, Journal of Insects as Food and Feed, Vol. 1 No. 3, pp. 215-225.

Rumpold, B.A. and Schlüter, O.K. (2013), “Potential and challenges of insects as an innovative source for food and feed production”, Innovative Food Science and Emerging Technologies, Vol. 17, January, pp. 1-11.

Schösler, H., Boer, J.D. and Boersema, J.J. (2012), “Can we cut out the meat of the dish? Constructing consumer-oriented pathways towards meat substitution”, Appetite, Vol. 58 No. 1, pp. 39-47.

Schutz, H.G. (1965), “Food action rating scale for measuring food acceptance”, Journal of Food Science, Vol. 30 No. 2, pp. 365-374.

Shelomi, M. (2015), “Why we still don’t eat insects: assessing entomophagy promotion through a diffusion of innovations framework”, Trends in Food Science and Technology, Vol. 45 No. 2, pp. 311-318.

Shipley, N.J. and Bixler, R.D. (2017), “Beautiful bugs, bothersome bugs, and FUN bugs: examining human interactions with insects and other arthropods”, Anthrozoös, Vol. 30 No. 3, pp. 357-372.

Sujan, M. and Dekleva, C. (1987), “Product categorization and inference making: some implications for comparative advertising”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 14 No. 1, pp. 372-378.

Tan, H.S.G., Van Den Berg, E. and Stieger, M. (2016), “The influence of product preparation, familiarity and individual traits on the consumer acceptance of insects as food”, Food Quality and Preference, Vol. 52, September, pp. 222-231.

Tan, H.S.G., Verbaan, Y.T. and Stieger, M. (2017), “How will better products improve the sensory-liking and willingness to buy insect-based foods?”, Food Research International, Vol. 92, February, pp. 95-105.

Tan, H.S.G., Fischer, A.R.H., Van Trijp, H.C.M. and Stieger, M. (2016), “Tasty but nasty? Exploring the role of sensory-liking and food appropriateness in the willingness to eat unusual novel foods like insects”, Food Quality and Preference, Vol. 48, March, pp. 293-302.

Tan, H.S.G., Fischer, A.R.H., Tinchan, P., Stieger, M., Steenbekkers, L.P.A. and Van Trijp, H.C.M. (2015), “Insects as food: exploring cultural exposure and individual experience as determinants of acceptance”, Food Quality and Preference, Vol. 42, June, pp. 78-89.

Tanaka, J.W. and Taylor, M. (1991), “Object categories and expertise: is the basic level in the eye of the beholder?”, Cognitive Psychology, Vol. 23 No. 3, pp. 457-482.

Van Huis, A. (2003), “Insects as food in Sub-Saharan Africa”, Insect Science and Its Application, Vol. 23 No. 3, pp. 163-185.

Van Huis, A., Van Gurp, H. and Dicke, M. (2014), The Insect Cookbook: Food for a Sustainable Planet, Columbia University Press, New York, NY.

Van Huis, A., Van Itterbeeck, J., Klunder, H., Mertens, E., Halloran, A., Muir, G. and Vantomme, P. (2013), “Edible insects: future prospects for food and feed security”, FAO forestry paper, Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations, Rome.

Verbeke, W. (2015), “Profiling consumers who are ready to adopt insects as a meat substitute in a Western society”, Food Quality and Preference, Vol. 39, January, pp. 147-155.

Zielińska, E., Baraniak, B., Karaś, M., Rybczyńska, K. and Jakubczyk, A. (2015), “Selected species of edible insects as a source of nutrient composition”, Food Research International, Vol. 77, January, pp. 460-466.

Corresponding author

Arnout R.H. Fischer is the corresponding author and can be contacted at: arnout.fischer@wur.nl

About the authors

Arnout R.H. Fischer is an Associate Professor in Consumer Behaviour with the Marketing and Consumer Behaviour Group of the Wageningen University, the Netherlands. He investigates how consumers respond to innovative food products and food technology from a social psychological perspective. His research includes public perception of consumers towards a range of innovations including insects as food, nanotechnology in food, genetic modification and cultured meat.

Bea Steenbekkers is an Assistant Professor in User-Centred Product Development with the Food Quality and Design Group of the Wageningen University, the Netherlands. She studies how consumer practices influence the final quality of a food product and how this can be related to innovative product design. Her main research interests include the user’s perspective on food quality.

Related articles