Introduction

All animals possess chemosensory systems which can detect chemical substances in their surroundings. The taste system of insects can detect a wide range of dissolved chemicals in potential food substrates and is important for assessing the quality of food resources (Dahanukar et al. 2005). About 50% of the ca. 1 million known insect species are plant feeders that are confronted with chemically diverse plant metabolites. There are many unpalatable compounds produced by plants used as food by insects in nature, and these co-occur in the same tissues as plant-produced nutrients (Hugentobler and Renwick 1994). These compounds have been named deterrents or suppressors, and elicit an aversive behavioral response in insects (Glendinning et al. 2001a). Therefore, taste discrimination among the complex chemical profiles encountered in plant tissues serves an essential function in host-plant selection of herbivorous insects (van Loon et al. 2008).

Lepidopteran larvae obtain contact chemical information from food by using taste neurons in three sets of taste sensilla located on their mouthparts: the labrum, the maxillary palps and the maxillary galea (Schoonhoven and van Loon 2002; Miles et al. 2005). The eight taste neurons innervating the lateral and medial sensilla styloconica on the maxillary galea play a decisive role in food selection behavior. Taste neurons in the two sensilla styloconica of several caterpillar species have been classified according to their best stimulus as sugar cell, water cell, salt cell, amino acid cell, inositol cell, deterrent cell, etc. (Schoonhoven and van Loon 2002). All herbivorous insects have deterrent cells, the activation of which leads to an aversive behavioral response (Bernays and Chapman 1987; Chapman and Bernays 1989; Dethier 1993; Bernays et al. 2000; Schoonhoven and van Loon 2002). It has been well documented that oligophagous insects such as Manduca sexta and Pieris rapae show behavioral adaptation to foods containing deterrent compounds (Schoonhoven 1969; Glendinning and Gonzalez 1995; Huang and Renwick 1995a; Renwick and Huang 1996; Glendinning et al. 2001a). In several cases, experience-induced changes in behavior could be related to the reduced sensitivity of deterrent cells (Schoonhoven 1969; van Loon 1990; Glendinning and Hills 1997; Glendinning et al. 1999, 2001a; Zhou et al. 2009). This has been interpreted as the result of “peripheral memory” located in the taste neurons (Schoonhoven and van Loon 2002). In addition, the experience-induced behavioral change upon exposure to certain compounds can generalize to other compounds that were not experienced before (Schoonhoven 1978; Glendinning et al. 1999). This phenomenon has been termed “cross-habituation” (Huang and Renwick 1995b, 1997). However, the mechanisms underlying such taste-mediated diet-induced behavioral plasticity are poorly understood.

The cotton bollworm Helicoverpa armigera (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae) is a typical polyphagous insect pest in China and many other countries, feeding on more than 200 crops in more than 40 plant families (Fitt 1989; Chen 1999; Zhao et al. 2006). In this study we report behavioral and electrophysiological responses of H. armigera caterpillars that experienced normal artificial diet or artificial diet containing two structurally different deterrent chemicals. We addressed two questions: (1) do habituation and “cross-habituation” occur in this generalist when exposed to deterrents during their larval development? (2) does exposure to deterrents affect gustatory responsiveness?

Materials and methods

Diets

Three types of diets were used: a normal artificial rearing diet (Wu and Gong 1997) and two experimental diets: the normal diet to which either strychnine or strophanthin-K was added at a concentration of 0.2 mM. Preliminary tests had shown that this concentration allowed sufficiently high survival rates (over 70%). The chemicals were added to the diet just after heating the agar, and the mixture was stirred vigorously for about 2–3 min (modified from Glendinning et al. 1999). The diets were cut into blocks to feed the caterpillars from neonates to the fifth instar.

Insects

Laboratory colonies of H. armigera were maintained as continuous cultures in the laboratory in an environmental chamber under a L16:D8 photo:scotoperiod, at a temperature of 25 ± 1°C and RH 60–70%. Larvae were reared individually in glass tubes plugged with cotton wool on the normal artificial diet. Adults were kept in cages and supplied with a 10% solution of honey in water. Larvae subjected to behavioral bioassays and electrophysiological tests were reared from neonate to fifth instar on normal artificial diet or diet containing one of the two chemicals. Caterpillars for behavioral assays were taken from the rearing diets at the end of the 4th instar in the non-feeding premoult stage and were deprived of food for about 6–8 h after moulting to the fifth instar and then tested (Tang et al. 2006).

Chemicals

Sucrose, inositol, strychnine-HCl and strophanthin-K were obtained from Sigma Chemical Co. (purity >97%). Strophanthin-K is a mixture of K-strophanthin-β, K-strophanthoside and other glucosides of cymarin (5% cymarin, 80% K-strophanthoside and 15% strophoside). For electrophysiological tests, strychnine and strophanthin-K were dissolved in 2 mM KCl, which served as control solution.

Behavioral bioassays

Two types of substrate were used in dual-choice disk tests: pepper fruit disks and cotton leaf disks. Cotton plants [Gossypium arboreum L., (Malvaceae) cultivar ‘Zhong-12’] were grown in the greenhouse of Wageningen University; pepper [Capsicum frutescens L. (Solanaceae), cultivar ‘JingTian-5’] was grown in the greenhouse of Daxing Agricultural Station in Beijing. In each assay, 20 caterpillars were tested. Strychnine and strophanthin-K were dissolved in 1% Tween-20 for use. Five microliters of solution of the test compounds was applied on a disk (78.5 mm2). Taking the pepper disk fresh weight/cotton leaf disk fresh weight into account, the concentration of strychnine and strophanthin-K tested was 2 mM. The experiments were carried out in a climatic chamber at a temperature of 25 ± 1°C. Larvae were placed individually in the middle of Petri dishes (diameter 12 cm) lined with moist filter paper. Four treated and four control disks were alternately arranged in an ABABABAB fashion around the circumference of the Petri dishes. This arrangement was modified after earlier studies to ensure that larvae had an equal chance of encountering both treated and control disks (Wang et al. 2004; Tang et al. 2006). Consumption was observed at hourly intervals thereafter. When ca. 50% of the control disks or treated disks had been eaten, which took 4–6 h, the disk remains were digitally scanned using a Hewlett-Packard flatbed scanner. Disk surface area was measured using Scion Image for Windows 4.03 (freeware, Scion Co. http://www.scioncorp.com/). The areas consumed were calculated by subtracting the remaining areas of leaf disks from the average area of three reference disks, which served as shrinkage controls (Messchendorp et al. 1996).

Electrophysiology

Caterpillars for electrophysiological tests were fifth instar larvae that had finished ecdysis 24–48 h earlier and had been starved for 2 h before testing (van Loon and Schoonhoven 1999). Three groups of larvae were tested: caterpillars reared from neonate to fifth instar on normal artificial diet or on diet containing one of the two chemicals. For each case, 12 replications were performed. Sucrose and inositol were tested at 10 mM on lateral and medial sensilla styloconica, respectively (Tang et al. 2000). For dose-response tests of the normal diet-reared caterpillars, the concentrations of the two chemicals tested were 0, 0.01, 0.1, 1 and 10 mM. For tests on caterpillars reared on the diets containing strychnine or strophanthin-K, the concentrations of the two chemicals were 0, 0.1, 1 and 2 mM. The tip-recording technique (Hodgson et al. 1955) was used to record responses to the different stimuli from the sensilla styloconica on the maxillary galea. Excised caterpillar heads were mounted on a silver wire electrode that was connected to the input of a pre-amplifier (Syntech Taste Probe DTP-1, Hilversum, The Netherlands). Stimulus solutions were filled into glass micropipettes with a tip diameter of c. 30 μm. Amplified signals were digitized by an A/D-interface (Syntech IDAC-4) and sampled into an Intel Pentium-based personal computer. Electrophysiological responses were quantified by counting the number of spikes in the first 1,000 ms after the start of stimulation. Spikes were counted visually by the experimenter with the aid of Autospike version 3.7 software (Syntech).

Statistical analysis

A feeding deterrent index (FDI) = 100(C − T)/(C + T) was calculated to quantify the strength of deterrence, where C represents the leaf disk area consumed from control disks, and T represents the leaf disk area consumed from treated disks.

Paired t tests were performed to analyze the disk consumption data. In electrophysiological tests, normal diet-reared caterpillars and deterrent-exposed caterpillars are independent samples; thus, a two-sample t test was performed to compare electrophysiological responsiveness of insects reared on different diets. All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS 13.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL).

Results

Behavioral assays

Larvae reared on normal artificial diet were strongly deterred by strychnine applied either on cotton leaf disks (FDI = 49.3; P < 0.01; Fig. 1a) or on pepper fruit disks (FDI = 39.4; P < 0.01; Fig. 1c). However, larvae reared on artificial diet containing strychnine did not discriminate between cotton leaf disks or pepper fruit disks treated with strychnine and disks treated with solvent only (FDI = 2.4, P > 0.05 on cotton leaf disk substrates; Fig. 1a; FDI = 3.2, P > 0.05 on pepper fruit disk substrates; Fig. 1c). Similarly, normal diet-reared caterpillars were sensitive to strophanthin-K either on cotton leaf disk substrates (FDI = 72.7; P < 0.001; Fig. 1b) or on pepper fruit disk substrates (FDI = 40.6; P < 0.01; Fig. 1d). In contrast, for caterpillars reared on the artificial diet containing strophanthin-K, the surface area consumed of cotton leaf disks or pepper disks did not differ significantly between control and strophanthin-K treated disks (FDI = 3.8, P > 0.05 on cotton leaf disks; Fig. 1b; FDI = −5.7, P > 0.05 on pepper fruit disks; Fig. 1d). Furthermore, caterpillars reared on artificial diet containing strychnine did not discriminate between pepper disks treated with strophanthin-K or with solvent (FDI = 2.0; P > 0.05; Fig. 1e). Likewise, caterpillars reared on artificial diet containing strophanthin-K were insensitive to strychnine (FDI = 6.6; P > 0.05; Fig. 1e).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Feeding preference behavior of fifth instar H. armigera larvae, reared on normal artificial diet, strychnine diet or strophanthin-K diet (indicated along the horizontal axis), on cotton leaf disks or pepper fruit disks in choice assays between control and the two chemicals. a Strychnine on cotton leaf disks b strophanthin-K on cotton leaf disks; c strychnine on pepper fruit disks; d strophanthin-K on pepper fruit disks; e strychnine and strophanthin-K on pepper fruit disks. N = 20 in all tests. Vertical lines represent standard errors. Asterisks indicate significant differences between treated and control disks according to the paired-samples t test (*P < 0.01; **P < 0.001)

Electrophysiological responses

Strychnine and strophanthin-K elicited relatively weak responses from the lateral sensilla styloconica. Response frequency of strychnine increased significantly from 0.01 to 0.1 mM; the response frequency of strophanthin-K increased significantly from 0.1 to 1 mM. The two chemicals elicited strong responses from the medial sensilla styloconica of the normal diet-reared caterpillars (Figs. 2a, b, 4b, e, respectively), demonstrating that this sensillum contains deterrent neurons that are considerably more sensitive to strychnine and strophanthin-K than neurons in the lateral sensillum. The deterrent neurons in the medial sensilla styloconica of caterpillars reared on artificial diet containing strychnine were significantly less sensitive to strychnine than in those of caterpillars reared on normal artificial diet when tested at 0.1, 1 and 2 mM (P < 0.05, Figs. 2c, 4c). Deterrent taste neurons of caterpillars reared on artificial diet containing strophanthin-K also produced significantly weaker responses to 0.1 and 1 mM strychnine than caterpillars reared on normal artificial diet (P < 0.01, P < 0.05, respectively, Figs. 2c, 4d). Similarly, H. armigera reared on artificial diet containing strychnine were significantly less sensitive to 1 and 2 mM strophanthin-K compared with caterpillars reared on normal artificial diet (P < 0.05, P < 0.001, respectively; Figs. 2d, 4f). Caterpillars reared on artificial diet containing strophanthin-K also had significantly weaker responses to 0.1, 1 and 2 mM strophanthin-K compared with caterpillars reared on normal artificial diet (P < 0.01, P < 0.05, P < 0.01, respectively; Figs. 2d, 4g). Furthermore, strychnine elicited responses from only one neuron in medial sensilla styloconica (Fig. 4b). However, strophanthin-K elicited responses from two neurons in medial sensilla styloconica, a more abundant larger amplitude spike type and a less abundant smaller amplitude spike type (Fig. 4e). The mixture of strychnine and strophanthin-K also elicited responses in which the same two spike types were observed, demonstrating that the neuron producing the more abundant and larger amplitude of the two spike types activated by strophanthin-K is the neuron responding to strychnine (Fig. 4b, e, h). Further analysis showed that the spike frequency of the neuron responsive to strychnine in the caterpillars reared on strychnine or strophanthin-K diet was significantly decreased when compared with normal diet-reared caterpillars (P < 0.01; Fig. 2e). However, the spike frequency of the neuron producing the less abundant smaller amplitude spikes did not differ significantly between caterpillars reared on normal diet and those reared on the diets containing the deterrents (P > 0.05; Fig. 2e). Sucrose elicited equally strong responses from the lateral sensilla styloconica irrespective of the diet to which the caterpillars had been exposed (Fig. 3). Similarly, responses of the inositol-sensitive neuron in the medial sensilla styloconica were equal for the caterpillars reared on the three different diets (Figs. 34).

Fig. 2
figure 2

a Dose-response curves for strychnine; spike frequencies (spikes/s; mean ± SE, n = 12) of the deterrent neurons in the two sensilla styloconica of H. armigera caterpillars reared on normal artificial diet from neonate to fifth instar. b Dose-response curves for strophanthin-K; spike frequencies (spikes/s; mean ± SE, n = 12) of the deterrent neurons in the two sensilla styloconica of H. armigera caterpillars reared on normal artificial diet from neonate to fifth instar. c Dose-response curves for strychnine; spike frequencies (spikes/s; mean ± SE, n = 12) of the deterrent neuron in medial sensilla styloconica of H. armigera caterpillars reared on normal artificial diet, artificial diet containing strychnine or artificial diet containing strophanthin-K during development to the fifth instar. d Dose-response curves for strophanthin-K; spike frequencies (spikes/s; mean ± SE, n = 12) of the deterrent neuron in medial sensilla styloconica of H. armigera caterpillars reared on normal artificial diet, artificial diet containing strychnine or artificial diet containing strophanthin-K during development to the fifth instar. e Spike frequencies (spikes/s; mean ± SE, n = 12) of the two different neurons activated by 1 mM strophanthin-K of H. armigera caterpillars reared on normal artificial diet, artificial diet containing strychnine or artificial diet containing strophanthin-K during development to the fifth instar. Bar values that have no letters in common differ significantly (t test, P < 0.05)

Fig. 3
figure 3

Spike frequencies (spikes/s; mean ± SE, n = 12) of the neuron in the lateral sensilla styloconica activated by 10 mM sucrose and the neuron in the medial sensilla styloconica activated by 10 mM inositol of H. armigera caterpillars reared on normal artificial diet, artificial diet containing strychnine or artificial diet containing strophanthin-K during development to the fifth instar

Fig. 4
figure 4

Exemplary recordings of electrophysiological activity in medial sensilla styloconica on the maxillary galea of fifth instar H. armigera reared on normal artificial diet, on artificial diet containing strychnine or on artificial diet containing strophanthin-K. Stimulus solutions tested and dietary experience: a Control (KCl 2 mM), normal artificial diet-reared caterpillars. b Strychnine 1 mM, normal artificial diet-reared caterpillars. c Strychnine 1 mM, caterpillars reared on artificial diet containing strychnine. d Strychnine 1 mM, caterpillars reared on strophanthin-K-containing diet. e Strophanthin-K 1 mM, normal artificial diet-reared caterpillars. f Strophanthin-K 1 mM, caterpillars reared on artificial diet containing strychnine. g Strophanthin-K 1 mM, caterpillars reared on artificial diet containing strophanthin-K. h Mixture of strychnine and strophanthin-K (both 1 mM), normal artificial diet-reared caterpillars. The onset of the stimulations occurred at the beginning of each trace. Arrows “1” and “2” point at two different spike types that strophanthin-K or its mixture with strychnine elicited from the medial sensilla styloconica. Arrow “1” refers to the more abundant larger amplitude spike type, arrow “2” refers to the less abundant smaller amplitude spike type. i Inositol 10 mM, normal artificial-diet reared caterpillars. j Inositol 10 mM, caterpillars reared on artificial diet containing strychnine. k Inositol 10 mM, caterpillars reared on artificial diet containing strophanthin-K

Discussion

Current theory holds that animals make decisions about the selection of mates, kin or food on the basis of pre-constructed recognition templates. These templates can be innate or acquired through experience (Sherman et al. 1997). The specialist caterpillar P. rapae reared on artificial diet was much less sensitive to several deterrents such as chlorogenic acid, cymarin, erysimoside and 2-O-β-d-glucosyl cucurbitacin E when compared with cabbage-reared caterpillars, and the compounds responsible for this difference have been identified in the wheat germ-fraction of the artificial diet as apigenin-based flavonoids (Renwick and Huang 1995; Huang and Renwick 1995b, 1997). Several studies documented that dietary experience influenced food selection behavior of other plant-feeding insect species (Jermy et al. 1967; Schoonhoven 1967; Bernays and Chapman 1987; Renwick and Lopez 1999; Glendinning et al. 2001a; Bernays and Singer 2005).

Strychnine is an alkaloid found in the seeds of the Strychnos nux-vomica tree. Strophanthin-K is a cardiac glycoside strophanthin mixture, obtained from the plant Strophanthus kombé. The two chemicals are not found in the known host plants of H. armigera. Therefore, they are “novel” compounds for the generalist H. armigera. In this study we used two different substrates in the behavioral experiments: cotton leaf disks and pepper fruit disks. Both cotton and pepper belong to the host plant range of H. armigera (Chen 1999). The advantage of pepper fruit disks as the substrate was that the chemical solution was taken up evenly by the disks, whereas on cotton leaf disks the compounds were most likely present in a concentrated surface layer. Nevertheless, we obtained similar behavioral results by using the two different substrates. We found that the normal artificial diet-reared H. armigera larvae were strongly deterred by both chemicals. However, after exposure to one of the two chemicals from neonate to the penultimate molt, caterpillars become habituated to both chemicals (Fig. 1a–e). This is a form of “cross-habituation,” which was defined as the phenomenon that dietary exposure of larvae to certain deterrent compounds can profoundly affect behavioral responses to other, chemically unrelated deterrents (Huang and Renwick 1995b).

The habituation observed may be based on several possible mechanisms. First, long-term contact of the deterrents could desensitize the taste neurons responding to deterrents, thus reducing the intensity of sensory input underlying the aversive behavioral response. The oligophagous Manduca sexta exhibited a reduced behavioral response to salicin after 72 h of dietary exposure to a salicin-containing diet. This behavioral adaptation to salicin was associated with a significant desensitization of the salicin-responsive taste cell in the lateral syloconic sensilla (Schoonhoven 1969). There are other examples of changes in the peripheral gustatory system as a result of dietary exposure, likewise associated with habituation. Manduca sexta fed an artificial diet with caffeine or salicin had reduced behavioral responsiveness to the same compounds. The experience-induced reduction in behavioral responsiveness to caffeine was associated with reduced peripheral taste responsiveness, but that to salicin was not, leading to the inference that experience-induced reduction in behavioral responsiveness to salicin was mediated centrally (Glendinning and Hills 1997; Glendinning et al. 1999, 2002). Compared with caterpillars of P. rapae reared on their host plant cabbage (Brassica oleracea L.), caterpillars reared on an artificial diet had a reduced sensitivity of deterrent neurons in medial sensilla styloconica to the structurally diverse chemicals chlorogenic acid, naringin and strychnine (Zhou et al. 2009). Caterpillars of P. brassicae, also an oligophagous insect on Brassicaceae, were less sensitive to 5 mM chlorogenic acid when reared on an artificial diet than cabbage-reared caterpillars (van Loon 1990). Second, herbivorous insects can increase their detoxification activity directed against certain plant toxins when they have a prolonged ingestion of such compounds. For example, Manduca sexta can induce the P450-mediated detoxification activities and thus increase the consumption of nicotine, the dominant allelochemical of its favorite host plants, Nicotiana species (Snyder and Glendinning 1996). In Spodoptera frugiperda, the increased consumption of the toxin indole 3-carbinol correlated with the induction of P450 enzymes (Glendinning and Slansky 1995). Third, chronic exposure to deterrents can result in synaptic plasticity in the processing of gustatory information in the central nervous system. The exposure to specific chemicals for a period of time might activate learning processes in the central nervous system, changing the behavioral responsiveness to taste stimuli (Sclafani 1991; Glendinning et al. 2001b).

In this study we tested if long-term exposure to two deterrents (strychnine and strophanthin-K) desensitized taste neurons in H. armigera larvae that showed excitatory responses to these allelochemicals. Caterpillars commonly have sensilla harboring deterrent-sensitive neurons in epipharyngeal sensilla and maxillary sensilla located on both the galea and the palp tips (Glendinning et al. 1998; Schoonhoven and van Loon 2002). The lateral and medial sensilla styloconica on the maxillary galea play a decisive role in detecting plant chemicals of the leaf interior (Schoonhoven and van Loon 2002); therefore, we focused on the latter sensilla in this study. Taste neurons in the epipharyngeal sensilla have the same specificity as the corresponding taste neurons in the styloconic sensilla of the maxillary galea in all cases studied and seem to control food swallowing rather than food selection (Schoonhoven and van Loon 2002). As the response of the medial sensilla styloconica to the two deterrents was very strong while that of the lateral sensilla styloconica was weak (Fig. 2), we focused on the deterrent neurons in the medial sensilla styloconica. The electrophysiological tests showed that the sensitivity of deterrent neurons in the medial sensilla styloconica to the two chemicals decreased significantly when the caterpillars were exposed from neonate until the penultimate molt (Figs. 2, 4). Furthermore, the mixture of strychnine and strophanthin-K showed that one deterrent neuron in the medial sensilla styloconica responded to both strychnine and strophanthin-K. Further analyses showed that this neuron, of the two neurons responding to strophanthin-K, was significantly less sensitive after long-term exposure to the two chemicals (Fig. 2e). Our interpretation of these findings is that “cross-habituation” could be tracked down to the same gustatory channel, and therefore to cross-desensitization. An alternative interpretation of our findings is a general decrease of sensitivity of the maxillary taste system. However, we demonstrate that the sensitivity of the neurons responsive to the feeding stimulants sucrose (in the lateral sensillum) and inositol (in the medial sensillum) did not change when the caterpillars experienced the diets containing the two deterrents (Figs. 3, 4), making the alternative interpretation unlikely. Our results showed that dietary experience affects the chemosensory sensitivity to the two chemically distinct classes of deterrent chemicals. Different scenarios could account for such a cross-desensitization: (1) the two chemicals possibly bind to the same promiscuous taste receptor protein although their chemical structures are quite different; (2) the two chemicals bind to different receptor molecules, but there could be a common step in the signal-transduction pathway occurring after Gr-ligand interactions, as proposed for the specialist P. rapae (Zhou et al. 2009).

Our combined results suggest that the reduced response at the behavioral level is at least partly explained by reduced taste neuron sensitivity. In addition, we found generalization (cross-habituation and cross-desensitization) between two structurally unrelated compounds. Our experimental methods do not allow a conclusion on the contribution of plasticity in the central nervous system or a possible role of increased detoxification activity to the behavioral changes observed. The clear link between reduced taste neuron activity and reduced taste-mediated behavioral responsiveness implies a causal effect of chemosensory plasticity on modified food selection behavior.